Archives for category: Artists

Littlmar_sml_1The SFEMS Weekly Calendar of Early Music —the Bay Area’s most comprehensive listing of historically informed early music events— has moved to the redesigned, searchable SFEMS website.

Find news, reviews, previews, artists bios, detailed program information and more at www.sfems.org

OLYMPUS DIGITAL CAMERAKnown for his dramatic and unique style and as a gifted orchestral colorist, Joseph Schwantner is one of the most prominent American composers today. He received his musical and academic training at the Chicago Conservatory and Northwestern University and has served on the faculties of the Juilliard School, Eastman School of Music, and the Yale School of Music.

Schwantner’s compositional career has been marked by many awards, grants, and fellowships, including the Pulitzer Prize in 1979 for his orchestral composition, Aftertones of Infinity, and several Grammy nominations. Among his many commissions is his Percussion Concerto, which was commissioned for the 150th anniversary season of the New York Philharmonic and is one of the most performed concert works of the past two decades.  Schwantner is a member of the American Academy of Arts and Letters. He says: “I was immediately drawn to the quartet’s poignant, intimate and expressive sound world, the evocative and hauntingly beautiful voice of contralto, Karen Clark and the poetry of Henry David Thoreau whose words so thoroughly capture the transcendental spirit of 19th-century New England life. Living and working in the high hills of rural New Hampshire, provided a synergy of poetry and music that formed an ideal union to help frame the mood, character and direction of this work.”

shapeimage_4Composer and performer on the viola da gamba, Roy Whelden has been called “a key figure in the world of new music” (Early Music America). He has been profiled as a composer and gambist on Minnesota Public Radio. He has made many recordings including the award winning Shock of the Old with American Baroque and San Francisco’s Composers’ Collective and Galax—music for viola da gamba (New Albion 059) as well as Like a Passing River (New Albion 072). He has performed and recorded with many internationally known ensembles, including Sequentia, the Boston Camerata, Musica Pacifica, American Baroque, and Ensemble Alcatraz. His instrument was made by Kazuya Sato in 1993, modeled on a variety of mid-17th century six string French viols. ■

John_Prescott02-1JOHN PRESCOTT received his M.A. and Ph.D. in musicology from U. C. Berkeley. Prior to this, he received his B.A. magna cum laude in Music and was elected to Phi Beta Kappa from Carleton College (MN). He has been the recipient of a number of academic honors, including the British Marshall Scholarship for two years of study at St. John’s College in Cambridge, England. He also studied at Oxford University’s Worcester College. Dr. Prescott has written extensively on the music of Handel. He wrote his doctoral dissertation on John Stanley, the 18th‐century blind organist, conductor, violinist and impresario. Dr. Prescott has taught music courses at U.C. Berkeley and music theory at The Crowden School (Berkeley CA), and was the musicologist for the San Francisco Elderhostel Arts and Humanities Program. He teaches music history courses for the Osher Life Long Learning institutes at U.C. Berkeley and at San Francisco State University. He gives pre‐opera lectures for Berkeley West Edge Opera and the Livermore Valley Opera Company. He also gives pre‐concert lectures for Philharmonia Baroque Orchestra.

Dead or Alive?: Improvisation in High Baroque Chamber Music

David Morris, viola da gamba and baroque cello;
Katherine Heater, harpsichord

david_morrisSunday, January 12, 2014 at 4:30PM
The Jazzschool
2087 Addison St., Berkeley
$15/$12
Reservations recommended as space is limited.

Buy Tickets >

Katherine HeaterImprovisation is at the heart of baroque music, even when composers give the performers fully fleshed-out melodies and information about the harmonic underpinnings of the music. Even with all of this black-and-white material facing modern performers, there’s still an expectation that the performer is supposed to give something of herself/ himself to every performance- something new, varied and unexpected. David Morris and Katherine Heater perform the music of Abel, Bach, Cirri, Marais, Telemann and others in a program that challenges listeners to figure out where the composer’s notes leave off and the performer’s new ideas begin.

David Morris is a member of The King’s Noyse, the Galax Quartet, Quicksilver and the New York State Baroque Ensemble. He has performed with Musica Pacifica, the Boston Early Music Festival Orchestra, Tragicomedia, Pacific Baroque Orchestra, Tafelmusik, the Boston Symphony Orchestra, Philharmonia Baroque Orchestra, American Bach Soloists, Musica Angelica, Seattle Baroque Orchestra, the Mark Morris Dance Company and Seattle’s Pacific Musicworks. Mr. Morris received his B.A. and M.A. in Music from U.C. Berkeley, and has been a guest instructor in early music performance-practice at UC Berkeley, UC Santa Cruz, the San Francisco Conservatory of Music, Mills College, Oberlin College, the Madison Early Music Festival and Cornell University. He has recorded for Harmonia Mundi, New Albion, Dorian, New World Records, Drag City Records, CBC/ Radio-Canada and New Line Cinema.

Katherine Heater, harpsichord, frequently plays with early music groups such as Musica Pacifica and Voices of Music. She has performed throughout the United States, including with Catacoustic Consort in Ohio, the Sun Valley Summer Symphony in Idaho and at the Berkeley Early Music Festival, the Bloomington Early Music Festival, and the Tropical Baroque Festival of Miami. She received an Arts Bachelor from the University of California, Berkeley in music and a Masters of Music in historical performance from Oberlin Conservatory. At the Sweelinck Conservatorium in Amsterdam Ms. Heater studied harpsichord with Bob van Asperen and fortepiano with Stanley Hoogland. Also an active teacher, Ms. Heater teaches harpsichord at UC Berkeley and privately. She has been director of the San Francisco Early Music Society’s Music Discovery Workshop and co-director of the SFEMS recorder and baroque workshops.

Dear Friend of Early Music,

Littlmar_sml_1Please Support SFEMS >

Since its founding nearly forty years ago, the mission of the San Francisco Early Music Society has been to support and nurture early music in the Bay Area, promoting the next generation of artists and working to build the audiences to receive them. In this time SFEMS has presented the world’s greatest exponents of historical performance on our concert series, offered annual summer workshops that attract students of all ages from around the globe, sustained an affiliate program to help all lovers of early music, whether recreational or professional, pursue their projects, incubated several of the Bay Area’s most prominent musical performing groups, and co-produced the biennial Berkeley Festival and Exhibition.

We are always seeking new and better ways to enrich our community through concerts, education, and co-operation with other organizations. This year, in partnership with the Jazzschool in Berkeley, SFEMS is producing a new series of informal Sunday afternoon concerts. These performances focus on the delights of improvisation, a common feature of both early music and jazz, and are introducing the world of historically informed music making to a new audience while challenging our traditional audience to experience early music in new and fascinating ways.

Also, this coming June, for the very first time, SFEMS will be the principal producer of the internationally acclaimed Berkeley Festival. Early Music America will join us in presenting its annual Exhibition & Marketplace of instruments, music, recordings, and accessories, as well as its acclaimed Young Performers Festival, and the Festival Fringe will provide a wealth of self-produced concerts by local and visiting performers.

We have begun preparations for the 2014 Festival, as board member Robert Cole has led us in assembling a marvelous series of concerts for the Festival week (see the listings at http://www.berkeleyfestival.org.). However, to fully meet the new leadership challenge of producing the Festival, SFEMS needs to raise $100,000 above and beyond our usual operating budget. The good news is that we have already made significant progress towards achieving this goal, and we are continuing to seek funding from a variety of sources. However, the bulk of our support will need to come—as it always does—from within our early music community, from people like you.

With this exciting new challenge before us, your support is more necessary, welcome and vital to our ongoing success than ever. If you have given to SFEMS in the past, please consider contributing at a higher level this year. If you have not given in the past, please consider making your first donation now. Every gift is very important to us, and every gift, no matter the size, will help SFEMS keep early music fresh and dynamic throughout the Bay Area. Please give generously!

Thank you so much for your support!
Sincerely,

John Signature1

John Phillips
President, SFEMS

Please Support SFEMS >

Part 2.
The Lyrebird, the Duck, and the Parrots

By JONATHAN HARRIS

lyrebird2 Bird songs likely have inspired human music for more than two thousand generations, at least since the Upper Paleolithic bird bone flutes that are our first tangible evidence of human musical culture. Whether birds are themselves musicians, intentionally creating the music we hear in their songs, there is little doubt birds perceive and take interest in human music, a fact noted by professional musicians, saints, and perspicacious bird keepers throughout history. That fact alone suggests they share with us a capacity for music.

That was the gist of last week’s essay. I want to explore a bit further now what I am calling this “capacity” for music, in light of recent scientific research and the personal experience of a couple SFEMS members.

A generation ago the idea that birds had significant self-awareness or interior life was beyond heretical; to suggest they had the intentionality to create or even appreciate music was ridiculous. Ornithologists interpreted birds as super-efficient biological machines, their systems optimized and miniaturized for flight, their brains pre-wired with tightly scripted behaviors adapted to a particular way of life. The small absolute size of a bird’s brain (a walnut for larger birds, a pea for smaller ones) was assumed to preclude intelligence. So did its neuroanatomy, lacking the developed cortex where higher cognitive functions take place in mammals. Birds’ vocalizations were explained as a set hard-wired calls and learned but mindless mimicry used to attract mates or defend territory. “Parroting,” needless to say, is a popular synonym for uncomprehending repetition.

Mainstream science in the 1970s was so dismissive of anthropomorphism and the idea of animal minds that Irene Pepperberg’s first grant application to study cognitive abilities of African Grey parrots was summarily rejected. When she proposed teaching her parrot to talk (at which they are adept) so it could answer questions directly, verbally (rather than using cumbersome, interactive computer screens), one reviewer wrote, “What has this woman been smoking?” Thirty-five years later, Pepperberg’s research with her parrot Alex had fundamentally transformed our notion of the “bird brain.” Alex proved to be a remarkably smart and chatty fellow. His cognitive abilities tested at the level of a 5–7 year-old human child; and he was able to communicate not just clearly and deliberately with human words, but use creativity and resourcefulness, sometimes even a little poetic imagination or irony, inventing new words and phrases to express novel concepts.

alex_parrotIn the past decade, scientists also have fundamentally revised their understanding of avian neuroanatomy. The mammal brain, with its capacity for higher cognitive functions located in the cortex, turns out to be not the pinnacle of evolution, the final, most advanced brain structure resulting from development along a linear path. As in other areas of biological evolution, it represents just one way those functions can be wired. The ancestors of birds and mammals diverged almost 300 million years ago, and as with the two groups’ respiration, digestion, reproductive systems, and vocalization anatomy, and many other things, their capacities for higher cognitive functions evolved independently to solve similar problems. In the case of birds, the neuroanatomical structures underlying higher cognitive functions got wired into different brain structures. As for size, it turns out that absolute brain size is not as good a predictor of intelligence as the size (mass) of the organ relative to total body size. And by that measure, birds are rather “large-brained” creatures. The ratio of a parrot’s brain to body weight, for instance, is comparable to that of some cetaceans and the larger apes; that of crows and ravens is in the same range as humans. New Caledonian crows, it should be noted, have shown great prowess in solving complex problems. In their natural habitat, they not only make tools for specific purposes; they have distinct, regional, “cultural” traditions of tool making in different parts of the island.

The idea that birds simply mimic sounds and throw them back without understanding does not withstand much scrutiny either. It is belied not only by the countless parrots who use words appropriately every day to express themselves; it also can be seen in the mimicry of songbirds. Philosopher David Rothenberg (Why Birds Sing) recounts the story of a naturalist trying to record an Albert’s Lyrebird in Australia. Lyrebirds are perhaps the best mimics in the world, assembling huge repertories of natural and human sounds, which they rattle off like perfect recordings of their original sources. They also are notoriously shy, and when this bird detected its human intruder, he threw out a litany of alarm calls from every species in his library. Note that he did not use his entire sound repertory, but intentionally chose that subset which would be most likely to be understood by his interloper. For examples of a Superb Lyrebird’s mimetic abilities, see this short sampler:

The meaning of bird songs often is reduced to their most familiar biological functions—defending territory or attracting a mate. Rothenberg’s story shows the limitation of reducing the meaning of any music—even a bird’s—to the automatic execution of a function, in this case defense of the bird’s territory. It tells us nothing about the lyrebird’s strategy, his “rhetoric,” if you will; nor the deftness, effectiveness, eloquence or beauty of his declamation, all of which of course would open the question of intention and control on the part of the bird-as-musician.

Imagine explaining human music this way. The function of most rock songs, e.g., is to express the desire to procreate. An enterprising scholar, using the model of birdsong, might hypothesize that guitar solos played at relatively higher pitch, higher volume, and with a higher number of notes per minute would indicate superior genes in the guitarist and be most likely to attract a mate. Indeed, this hypothesis might be tested in the field by searching out and counting the offspring of aging guitarists scattered in various cities around the US. However, such analyses would tell us nothing about what their music meant. That a song fills a particular function says remarkably little about its content, rhetoric, or quality; it does not distinguish the guitar solos of a Jimi Hendrix from a Nigel Tufnel.

My own experience living with birds (canaries, cockatiels and blue-crowned conures) for the past several years suggests that all of them vocalize for many reasons, not only for courtship or territorial self proclamation, but sometimes for camaraderie or comfort, and sometimes simply to enjoy the play of sound. They all make a wide variety of sounds, respond to and imitate each other and us humans. All 4 of our parrots themselves initiate or eagerly join sound imitation games, the conures playing with words or noises, the cockatiels with whistling.

The cockatiels and canary also respond to environmental background sounds with whistling. The gurgling of tap water, the whine of a heater or appliance, or the hum a passing street sweeper, can become for them drones over which to improvise. The birds also respond to one another, both imitating each other’s sounds and sometimes playing off them to form more elaborate musical dialogues. Here are a couple short videos illustrating such dialogues. One is a simple exchange between a cockatiel and canary, prompted at first by the hum of a passing street sweeper, which leads to a more heated peak with each also responding to the other before they both relax.

The second is a more elaborate trading of riffs back and forth between the canary (on screen) and one of the conures (off screen). The thematic material is more complex, but it is possible to hear the conscious exchange of chatter between birds and the imitation of motives

The conures, curiously, respond less to ambient sounds than to music itself. Sometimes music prompts their excited screams or squawks; often they dance. Many parrot species, it turns out, move to music (the documentary Wild Parrots of Telegraph Hill has a short clip of the cherryhead conure Mingus dancing:

The kind of music is surprisingly varied. While they enjoy lively, highly rhythmic songs, they will respond to almost any music if they are in the mood. I have seen one of the birds, Paco, dance to vocal warm-ups, bobbing his head with each change of pitch. That shows an ability to hear melodic rhythm, to hear the rhythm implicit in the change of pitch, not just to feel a beat. I have seen the other conure, Joey, dance to Ligeti’s cloud music. That suggests just as strongly that—even without rhythm or even clear notes or pitches—he has an intuition of Ligeti as music, which in turn means that music is for him a category, an acoustical object, a distinct “kind” of sound; moreover, his intuition of what fits in the category of music is similar to ours.

A clear line scientists drew for many years to distinguish the musical capabilities of humans from those of animals was “entrainment,” the ability to synchronize body movement with a beat. As recently as 5 or 6 years ago, luminaries at the level of Oliver Sachs (Musicophilia) and Daniel Levitin (This is Your Brain on Music) publicly declared no animal could do what a 3-year-old human child could do. That proclamation too is proving incorrect. In 2007, neuropsychologist Aniruddh Patel (Music, Language, and the Brain) became aware of a YouTube video showing a Medium Sulphur Crested Eleanora Cockatoo named Snowball dancing to the Back Street Boys

His taste was deplorable, but of course he was a parrot. Nevertheless, Patel was impressed by how well he followed the beat and arranged to test the bird’s ability. It turned out Snowball was quite deliberately synchronizing his dance moves to the beat; he could and would vary his pulse to follow changes in the music’s tempo. Patel and colleagues searched YouTube and the Web for other examples of entrainment and found a dozen other animals, mostly parrots but some mammals, including an elephant, that shared this capability.

Longtime SFEMS members, especially those who attended the first, more rustic and rural summer workshops may remember Angene Feves’ duck, Laura Soave. Angene, an internationally renowned historical dance expert, taught baroque and Renaissance dance at our summer workshops during their first decade; she had several ducks, one of whom she considered special and smuggled into the workshops for a few years. Angene considered Laura to be not only quite smart and empathic; she also claimed she had musical abilities. She once joked the duck could realize figured bass. By which she meant she would both dance and quack along, following the music. What makes this story interesting is that it is not about a songbird or a parrot but a duck, a bird with very primitive vocal apparatus capable of making only limited sounds. What it suggests is that the ability to recognize and appreciate music is independent of the ability to produce it. It also suggests it is widespread in birds. The ancestors of ducks, chickens, and their relatives diverged from the other birds over 110 million years ago.

Why do birds have this great enthusiasm for music? One possible answer is that music is something tangible and perceptible in the world as much as it is created or invented. Our brains are somehow wired to grasp a certain kind of order in sound (Patel now claims that with respect to their abilities to process and interpret sound, human minds are more similar to those of birds than to our closest primate relatives).

Luther’s statement about music being a largesse of God thus may have been close to the truth than we have been prepared to grant. Luther’s greatest musical follower, J.S. Bach, considered music a science. Indeed, his music often feels like the solution to mathematical or logical puzzles, with Bach searching for relationships that existed outside himself. In some sense all composers must do this, though their probings may be less profound or sophisticated than Bach’s. And I suspect birds likewise are wired to recognize something that is out there in the universe, a harmony of the spheres, if you will. Consider how a child who never has been allowed to have sugar reacts to its first piece of birthday cake. Hard-wired bliss—a gastrodelic experience. It is a real sensation and a perception, grounded in the external world as much as in our own minds. That is why Native Americans reacted so strongly to western polyphony when they first heard it in the 16th century, because it was a sugar rush to their minds and bodies in ways (harmony and counterpoint) they were wired to grasp but had not themselves (fully) conceived. Western listeners have had similar reactions to African polyrhythms on first hearing. Almost everyone has had the thrill of neophilia at some point on hearing a new musical genre or even a new piece of music that moves us powerfully in ways we had never imagined. It is that same quality I believe excites birds and makes them interested in our music. It is what Joey and Snowball perceive, and what Angene Feves saw in her duck and Mozart in his starling, and what caused such delight in the birds who gathered around Landini’s head to hear him extemporize on a hot summer’s day 600 years ago.

Don’t miss:

Frances Feldon and Galax Quartet in “Wingin’ It”

FFeldon_67-1Sunday, November 17 at 4:30PM
The Jazzschool
2087 Addison St., Berkeley
$15/$12
Reservations recommended as space is limited.
For tickets: http://jazzschool.org/event/frances-feldon-galax-quartet/.

Touchstone

Songs, dances and improvisations drawn from medieval to modern modal traditions

SUNDAY, OCTOBER 6, 4:30PM
The Jazzschool
2087 Addison St., Berkeley
510-845-5373 and More Info and Tickets >

Shira Kammen, bowed strings
Peter Maund, percussion

Nouvelle Amor…based on troubador/cantiga melody
Natchez…based on melodies from the Cantigas de Santa Maria
Ben com’ aos que van per mar…based on Cantiga 49
Dame a Vous…Guillaume de Machaut (c. 1300-1377)

——————————————

Stantipes…based on Medieval English melodies

——————————————

Salinas…based on Renaissance melodies by Francisco de Salinas (1513 – 1590)
Ronde de Saint Vincent…trad. Breton
Nou Welcome Somer…text Chaucer, from the Parliament of Foweles, music S.Kamme
Ar ne Kuth…based on a Medieval English lament, by Margriet Tindemans

——————————————

Cominciamento di Gioia…14th c. Italian Anon, MS 29987
Improvisation…Oct 6, 2013
Ecco la Primavera…Francesco Landini (c. 1325 or 1335 – September 2, 1397)
Saltarello…14th c. Italian Anon, MS 29987

touchstone1-1

JazzSchool-logoThe Jazzschool in Berkeley and The San Francisco Early Music Society have established an exciting new affiliation that we anticipate will delight both our audience and our local musicians. Beginning October 6th early music lovers and jazz music lovers will have the opportunity to join together to celebrate the improvisational kinship of the two genres.The Jazzschool will host monthly Sunday afternoon concerts of early music, each concert featuring members of our community performing on period instruments and expounding the common improvisational thread (what philosopher David Rothenberg called “sudden music”) that links early music and jazz.
Read about the project in the SFEMS Bay Area Early Music News

Shira Kammen and Peter Maund

current bio pic SKA new concert series co-produced by SFEMS and the Jazzschool begins on October 6 with Touchstone, an ensemble featuring Shira Kammen and Peter Maund, and the organizers could scarcely have provided a more auspicious beginning. The two have been frequent collaborators over the past 30 years, most notably with the late, great Ensemble Alcatraz.PM_Frame_Drums

Besides their legendary work in medieval music, Kammen and Maund are fluent in the languages of many musical cultures, from Ireland to Iran, and much of what lies between. They have played with a wide range of semi-improvisatory groups representing these traditions, as well as crossovers, such as the jazzclassical-Jewish roots group Davka, with which Peter performed, and  The Lost Mode, with which Shira appeared on the SFEMS series last year.

SUNDAY, OCTOBER 6, 4:30PM
The Jazzschool
2087 Addison St., Berkeley
510-845-5373 and More Info and Tickets >

Follow

Get every new post delivered to your Inbox.